Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Effectiveness of communication of spatial engineering information through 3D CAD and 3D printed models

Abstract

Background

Poor engineering information provided to construction crews results in inefficient communication of design, leading to construction rework, disputes, and lower worker morale. The resulting errors, omissions, and misinterpretations indicates that a significant opportunity exists to improve the traditional documentation of engineering information that craft professionals use to complete their work. Historically, physical three dimensional (3D) models built by hand provided 3D physical representations of the project to assist in sequencing, visualization, and planning of critical construction activities. This practice has greatly diminished since the adoption of three dimensional CAD (computer-aided design) and BIM (building information modeling) technologies. Recently, additive manufacturing technologies have allowed for three dimensional printing of 3D CAD models.

Methods

The effectiveness of 2D drawings, a 3D computer model, and a 3D printed model in delivering engineering information to an end-user are scientifically measured.

Results

The 3D printed model outperformed the 2D drawings and 3D computer interface in productivity and workload measures.

Conclusions

A physical model has the ability to improve communication of spatial design for certain tasks. This could lead to improved productivity, reduction of errors, and better quality for construction tasks. This paper's primary contribution to the body of knowledge is that it identifies how different mediums of engineering information impact the communication of spatial engineering information.

Background

While the U.S. construction industry comprises 5.4% of the overall U.S. Gross Domestic Product (GDP) (Bureau of Economic Analysis [2013]; and Huesman et al. [2013]), it lags the rest of the economy in terms of productivity measurement and improvement (Goodrum et al. [2002]). Due to changes in real output and differences in accounting procedures, there is no isolated industry level measure of labor productivity (Bureau of Labor Statistics [2012]), although recently the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics has developed an aggregate multifactor productivity measure for the combined agricultural, mining, and construction industries (Bureau of Labor Statistics [2013]). Regardless of this recent advancement, an isolated productivity index for the U.S. construction industry makes it difficult to track progress, benchmark, and measure the effects of governmental and industrial policies across the construction industry.

However, at a project level, productivity figures are more diligently kept, although still inconsistently company to company. With profit margins near 3%, firms must do what they can to track their performance and make necessary changes (Cooper and Lee [2009]). A major contributor to the overall productivity is execution of workface practices. If construction practitioners are not equipped with the necessary tools, information, materials, and equipment to effectively perform their tasks, the productivity of the project will be negatively affected. This research presents insights into how different mediums, specifically two dimensional drawings, three dimensional computer models, and three dimensional physical models, of engineering information influence the performance of task execution.

Information delivery and its effect on construction productivity

Traditionally, two dimensional drawings (commonly referred to as blueprints) are the spatial and technical communication media for all project participants (Gould and Joyce [2013]). Drawings are presented in a variety of formats including plan views, elevations, detailed sections, and isometrics. Individual drawings are often scaled, list dimensions, and frequently reference other sheets to help provide the user a representation of the final design intent from all viewpoints.

One of the main challenges in processing engineering graphical information is that the design may be well intended, but for a variety of reasons, the message received differs from the original intent. This process of creating a message, disseminating, and then processing essentially describes the well published theory of the linear standard communication process. Many models have been created to describe the process with varying stages, however, the essential elements are outlined in Figure 1 (Shannon and Weaver [1949]; Schramm [1954]; and Berlo [1960]).

Figure 1
figure 1

Standard model of communication.

In this standard, linear model of communication, the sender must encode their interpretation of the desired end user information. This interpretation is then translated in the message medium, whether it is verbal or non-verbal. Receivers must decode the message into their individual interpretation, where the final message is processed and understood. However, the intermediary steps of encoding the message, the creation of the message, and decoding the message increases the potential for noise that can affect the ultimate outcome of the communication. The message channel is the actual flow of the message, verbal or nonverbal, from the sender to the receiver. In the linear model, there is very little, if any, feedback from the receiver to the sender, where ultimately only a downstream process occurs (Shannon and Weaver [1949]; Schramm [1954]; and Berlo [1960]). In the communication of construction documents, poor design and poor design communication are two distinct opportunities for errors (Eckert and Boujut [2003]). Poor design is the result of errors made by designers in the encoding process, while poor design communication are errors made in the interpretation or decoding of the message.

Errors made through poor design have been studied and identified as having an impact on project performance. Several studies discuss errors in the design outputs concluding that many practitioners feel information delivery is a deterrent to efficiently performing their job (Goodrum and Dai [2006]; Dai et al. [2009a]; Dai et al. [2009b]; Mourgues and Fischer [2008]; and Rojas [2008]). The main inefficiencies from drawing management according to a survey of construction craft workers are due to errors in the drawing, availability of the drawings, slow management response to questions, legibility, and omission of necessary information on the documents (Goodrum and Dai [2006]; Dai et al. [2009a]; and Dai et al. [2009b]). In a focus group study, Dai et al. ([2005]) found that craft professionals believe 32% of their negative productivity is due to inaccuracy and poor quality of information, with individuals claiming that “some drawings could not be physically built”. Rojas ([2008]) discusses inefficiencies from design drawings ultimately leading to increased rework on the project. Borcherding and Samelson ([1980]) found that rework was one of the three most significant drivers to poor productivity and decreased morale, oftentimes as a result of poor engineering information design. Haas et al. ([2011]) found that design, engineering, instruction, and monitoring accounted for 29.08% of the total amount of rework on an analysis of records from the industrial sector. Supervisors and foremen then become focused on correcting engineering errors and rework instead of planning future work and focusing on crew performance. In the highway construction sector, an analysis of change orders showed that omissions of information led to a 4.53% increase in original contract amount among a sample of roadway projects (Taylor et al. [2012]). With 40% of the total construction cost being in direct and indirect craft labor, there is a need to maximize efficiency and reduce non-value adding activities of the workers (Haas et al. [2011]).

Similarly, poor design communication can lead to errors and confusion in task execution. Some of the issues result from the difference in the message intended versus the message received. The format and intent of drawings is easier to comprehend by the designer that creates the drawing than it is for the contractor and his/her workforce that has to interpret the message (Emmitt and Gorse [2003]). This problem is magnified when the contractor must reference several different drawings to understand the design intent for a particular building element. Further, different symbols and terminology used by various designers can also lead to confusion and complications (Emmitt and Gorse [2003]). Mourgues and Fischer ([2008]) argue that communication of project information to the workface is ineffective and can negatively impact quality, safety, and productivity. Studying the understanding and communication of design throughout all project participants can greatly improve project outcomes by increasing project morale, improving productivity, and reducing rework.

Physical models and additive manufacturing

Physical scale models have had several uses in the construction industry, for tasks such as sequencing of concrete and steel or for planning rigging, reinforcing steel, or excavation operations. Oglesby et al. ([1989]) surveyed managers who concluded that physical models were a useful tool for planning and communications, and that modeling pays for itself easily. However, no direct benefit was quantified. Physical models, as a supportive piece of information delivery, can be a useful information tool. A study estimates that labor costs can be reduced by 25% minimizing productivity losses on indirect work through a detailed execution planning strategy (O'Brien et al. [2011]). The use of physical models in construction was prevalent historically, but their current use is diminished (Emmitt and Gorse [2003]). The development of 3D CAD technologies is cited as one of the main causes behind the fading use of physical scale models (Emmitt and Gorse [2003]).

3D printing technologies have developed and advanced to the point where 3D CAD models can be effectively printed (Breen et al. [2003]; and Simondetti [2002]). These modeling techniques are defined to be “the process of joining materials to make objects from 3D model data, usually layer upon layer” (Buswell et al. [2007]; and Lim et al. [2012]). Having the ability to create physical models through a printing process presents an opportunity to support traditional information delivery with an accurate, scaled representation of a three dimensional space. There are many models of 3D printers with similar technologies. The printers work by essentially building up an object with individual thin layers of a material, such as plastics or starch. ABS (acrylonitrile butadiene styrene) plastics or PLA (polylactic acid) plastics are two of the more common plastic outputs. A 3D printed model was selected to represent a physical model to test its applicability as a format for information display. Ultimately, the means to achieve the end result of a physical model are not relevant to the outcomes of the study, as the primary objective was to test a physical model as opposed to blueprints or a CAD model.

Mental workload

A critical cognitive component to the design of information delivery is in the mental workload requirements. Assuming that everyone has a fixed maximum cognitive capacity, mental workload is the amount of mental resource required compared to the total resources available to that person (Carswell et al. [2005]). An effective method of information delivery should reduce the mental workload requirements while allowing the necessary information to be conveyed. Typically, this involves reducing the amount of time the user must retain the information in their working memory and reduce the irrelevant, distracting mental operations that may occur. Ultimately, a desirable level of mental workload leads to improvements in design communication as information is able to be retained.

Research in cognitive design has identified three classes of mental workload metrics used to evaluate the outcome of the study. The classes are physiologic, secondary task, and subjective measures (Carswell et al. [2005]). Physiologic measures use indirect measures of mental workload by studying ocular and cardiac responses. These physiologic responses have a relationship with cognitive activity in the brain. Increased cognitive activity has been found to result in small increases in pupil dilation, slowing blinking patterns, more consistent heart rates, and heightened heart rates (Carswell et al. [2005]). Secondary task measures look to identify the remainder of the mental workload, which is not occupied by performing the desired task. These secondary tasks are developed for certain applications such as aviation and high-demand environments. For this study, objective and subjective measures are used as they are readily available, universally accepted, nonintrusive, and the simplest to administer.

One of the most widely used standardized subjective measures of mental workload is the National Aeronautics and Space Administration Task Load Index (NASA-TLX) (Hart and Staveland [1988]). This tool has been used to measure mental workload in over 1,200 studies since its inception (Hart [2006]). Although its use is widespread across fields (e.g. aviation, transportation, and other display oriented fields), its use within construction research has been limited. Mitropoulos and Memarian ([2013]) investigated task demands in masonry work using the NASA-TLX to identify factors affecting activity performance and propose strategies to improve performance.

However, there are drawbacks to current subjective measures. The subjects must self-evaluate their performance and their cognitive capacity. Response bias could factor into the results if the subjects are stakeholders in the study (Carswell et al. [2005]). For instance, if conducting this study with a veteran journeyman electrician, he or she may be inclined to prefer the traditional 2D drawing set because of their familiarity with the medium.

Methods

This research focuses on spatial information format as an initial mechanism for improving understanding and communication of design and evaluates three different formats using several measures. The ability to evaluate cognitive abilities of practitioners in using various information delivery formats requires defined performance metrics. In a discussion of construction communication deliverables, Emmitt and Gorse ([2003]) suggest that information formats must yield quick, simple, and easily interpretable results. Using those guidelines along with the cognitive principles and measures previously discussed, a series of evaluations have been created for assessment.

Cognitive task

The main portion of the experiment is a building task using scale model elements to recreate a structure based on given information. The basis of design for the experiment's model must be simple enough to solely capture the cognitive aspects of spatial information processing, yet complex enough to where there is difficulty and mistakes can be made. If the model is too complex, the design would bias the results away from understanding the performance of medium. If the model is too simple, there will not be a differentiation in the measures. The structure design was created through a standard set of two dimensional construction drawings (blueprints), a three dimensional computer aided design (CAD) model, and a physical model created by a three dimensional printer from the CAD model. These forms of media were selected as they are typical display formats and are accepted as the forms used to communication spatial design (Emmitt and Gorse [2003]). Subjects were presented with information in one of the three formats, from a randomized sequence, and then instructed to build the structure from simple building elements. The same structure was used for each experiment but the participants were not told that the three structures were identical. To control and identify a potential learning curve, the sequence that the mediums were presented was rotated. The subjects were free to manipulate the models as needed, and the order in which the subjects received the models was randomized and were available to consult throughout the experiment. Understanding that the subjects worked with the same model, the authors wished to identify if there was a learning curve present. Due to the scope of this paper and length restrictions, this phenomena was studied and reported in another publication (Dadi et al. [2014]), where the authors examined the cognitive outcomes of the experiment in significant depth. The objective of the previous manuscript was to study the cognitive outcomes of various information formats, where this current manuscript identifies the performance of the formats in many key areas such as speed to completion, productivity, and errors made. Samples of the 2D drawings are illustrated in Figures 2 and 3. The 3D CAD model is pictured in Figures 4 and 5. Finally, the physical model can be seen in Figure 6. Figures 7 and 8 show the building elements, the Tekton Tower Girder and Panel Building Set by Bridge Street Toys, before and after model creation respectively. The amount of building elements in the set outnumbered the pieces necessary to complete the model.

Figure 2
figure 2

2D plan view of experiment model.

Figure 3
figure 3

2D elevation view of experiment model.

Figure 4
figure 4

3D computer model top view.

Figure 5
figure 5

3D computer model isometric view.

Figure 6
figure 6

3D printed model isometric view (shown next to standard size playing card for scale).

Figure 7
figure 7

Scale model building elements disassembled (shown next to standard size playing card for scale).

Figure 8
figure 8

Scale model building elements appropriately assembled (shown next to standard size playing card for scale).

Sample group

Practitioners were recruited to provide a sample of individuals that regularly use design and construction drawings for the purposes of field installation of the final design intent. The subjects from this sample group were recruited from engineering and construction firms through an e-mail invitation with a range of experience from one year to over thirty years. The scope of the study was presented to management and executive level employees at several companies throughout the state of Kentucky. In this notification of study, the employees were asked to make engineers and craft workers available to complete the study, and the companies provided 26 individuals to complete the experiment. Job titles included carpenter, laborer, electrical, and mechanical foreman, as well as project engineer and design engineer. The study attracted the participation of 26 subjects and are outlined in Table 1.

Table 1 Sample demographics

Five-minute rating analyses have been performed on construction field projects to measure the effectiveness of a construction crew and identify sources of delay, travel, and other non-effective work activities (Oglesby et al. [1989]). For this experiment, a five-minute rating yielded the percent of the task time that was spent on direct or effective work and on non-effective work or rework. The percentage can be applied to the overall time to completion to give the amount of time spent on each activity category. The data yields effective work percentages of each information delivery format. To conduct a five-minute rating, a time sheet was prepared and divided into subsets of time and then columns for notation of the activity classification. The classification categories are direct work, indirect work, rework, and delay due to rework. Direct work is defined as any physical building of the model towards the final product. Indirect work is defined as any activities performed towards the end result that is not physically building the model. This includes time getting familiar with the building elements, and manipulating and processing the information delivery format. Rework includes any disassembling or reassembling of a previously built portion of the model. Finally, delay due to rework includes time spent reprocessing the information delivery medium after rework occurs.

To measure the cognitive performance of the subjects, the National Aeronautics and Space Administration Raw Task Load Index (NASA-rTLX) was utilized to measure mental workload. Mental workload is a measure of the amount of mental resources required to complete a task compared to the total amount of mental resources available to that individual (Carswell et al. [2005]). Often, the desired workload imposed by a task has a balance. Too much mental workload and the user may not have the capacity to maintain a proper level of performance. Too little mental workload and the user may not have the focus and diligence required to complete the task appropriately (Hart and Staveland [1988]; and Mitropoulos and Memarian [2013]).

The NASA-TLX workload measure is a multidimensional tool that rates responses in accordance to six factors: 1) mental demand, 2) physical demand, 3) temporal demand, 4) effort, 5) performance, and 6) frustration (Table 2). The traditional NASA-TLX obtains pairwise weights for each of the six factors prior to identifying a scaled response for the factors. These sub-factors then combine based on the weight from the pairwise scores to form a composite workload score. A derivative of the NASA-TLX is the NASA Raw Task Load Index (NASA-rTLX) that does not weight the subscales by their pairwise comparisons, resulting in a composite workload score from an average of the subscales. Several studies have found a strong correlation between the NASA-TLX and NASA-rTLX, which lends towards the adoption of the simpler NASA-rTLX tool (Moroney et al. [1995]; Moroney et al. [1992]; and Byers et al. [1989]). This study utilized the NASA-rTLX tool to measure workload for the participants. Table 2 provides a description of the NASA-rTLX factors and the measurement scale.

Table 2 NASA-rTLX factors and descriptions ( Hart and Staveland, [1988])

Statistical analysis procedure

A key statistical measure used in the study's experiments is the analysis of variance (ANOVA) procedure. ANOVA models seek to test whether there is a difference between means of several populations (Dielman [2005]; Fellows and Liu [2008]). The often performed procedure estimates statistically significant differences between the means through an F value, while also measuring the amount of variation in the dependent variable that is explained by the independent variables (η2) (Dielman [2005]; and Fellows and Liu [2008]).

For this study, the three populations tested are individuals completing the experiment using the two dimensional drawing set, individuals completing the experiment using the three dimensional computer model, and individuals completing the experiment using the three dimensional printed, physical model.

To quantify these statistically significant differences, there are several post hoc tests available to compare multiple means. The original post hoc test was Fisher's Least Significant Difference (LSD) test. This test compared multiple means through a series of t-tests. However, no adjustment was made to the error rate for the comparisons. In the assumptions of a t-test, the sampling distribution was intended for only one test. When multiple comparisons are made, the true alpha value for significance is lower than 0.05, which is the value assumed in the LSD test (Dielman [2005]). Another, more reasonable post hoc test is the Bonferroni method. Bonferroni uses t tests to perform pairwise comparisons but sets the critical alpha value as the experiment-wise error rate divided by the total number of tests. This corrects for the effect that multiple tests has on the tested t value (Dielman [2005]).

Results and discussion

For the model with all subjects, there were 26 participating subjects, resulting in 77 complete building experiments. One individual could not complete a third model, which prevented the sample size from reaching 78. Each subject completed the task with all three model types in a randomized order. Table 3 reports the average results across all participants for the four key obtained variables.

Table 3 ANOVA results: model type by dependent variables, all subjects

The physical model had preferred outcomes in time to complete the experiment, composite mental workload, and direct work rate. The 2D drawing set resulted in the lowest rework rate percentage. Of significance, individuals spent more time interpreting the information from the 3D computer interface, whereas the physical model required little processing time. As with all information processing, the work task will demand the optimal display format.

Preferences for engineering and construction tasks

In a post-test questionnaire, the subjects were presented with real work scenarios that require the use of spatial information and asked which format should be used to complete the task. The scenarios were selected based on previous research on the advantages of display format (Boyer and Wickens [1994]; Cockburn and McKenzie [2002]; Hickox and Wickens [1999]; and St John et al. [2001]). Table 4 summarizes the desired information traits. Relative positioning presents better in two dimensions, as the specific planar dimensions can be focused on, and the third, and unnecessary dimension, is eliminated. When projective ambiguity is a concern, a two dimensional format is superior. Projective ambiguity exists when three dimensions are recreated in a two dimensional format, resulting in a distorted third dimension. 3D displays better represent shape understanding as a full 360° viewing angle can be achieved. In a similar fashion, a 3D display allows the user to focus on a plane while still having quick reference to a third dimension. When understanding a layout or terrain, a profile view can be accessed while also having the depth (or width, depending on the chosen profile) dimension readily available. Finally, a 3D display allows for depth cues to be referenced. This means that a 2D sheet can be studied while also having the third (depth) dimension represented to give a point of reference for depth and location.

Table 4 2D versus 3D display comparisons

There were four tasks presented in the post-test questionnaire to identify preferences of the practitioners. The tasks were:

  • You are a structural steel subcontractor and need to plan and present an erection sequence, which information delivery format (s) would you use to complete the task (2D, 3D Interface, Physical Model)?

  • If you are calculating the necessary cubic yards of concrete for an upcoming slab pour, which information delivery format (s) would you use to complete the task (2D, 3D Interface, Physical Model)?

  • If you are a mechanical, electrical, or plumbing (MEP) engineer and need to design piping runs with sufficient access space, which information delivery format (s) would you use to complete the task (2D, 3D Interface, Physical Model)?

  • If you are estimating the quantity of earthwork that will have to be cut and/or filled on a project, which information delivery format (s) would you use to complete the task (2D, 3D Interface, Physical Model)?

In a construction setting, a structural steel erection plan requires an understanding of relative positioning, as it involves coordination of the construction of steel shapes in two directions or dimensions. Therefore an ideal information format choice would be the 2D drawings. Calculating the required yardage of concrete for a future placement event requires an understanding of the shape and the ability to measure distances. Shape understanding presents well in three dimensions, which would point towards the 3D computer model or the physical model. Being that distances are represented and automatically calculated in the computer software, the 3D computer model provides the best representation. MEP runs are typically associated with having sufficient access and coordination between the trades to fit the pipes in the allowable space provided. This requires depth cues and shape understanding without projective ambiguity. The depth cues and shape understand lends itself towards a 3D model, while projective ambiguity concerns lead the user towards a 2D representation. However, a physical model provides the necessary depth cues and shape understanding in a proper and efficient 3D representation. Finally, estimating the quantity of earthwork for cut and fill requires project information and layout understanding of the terrain. Similar to the concrete placement scenario, a 3D computer has the necessary display, information, and calculating tools to complete the task.

Having reviewed the scenarios and proper information format displays, Figures 9, 10, 11, and 12 display the preferences for practitioners to complete the steel erection plan, concrete placement, MEP coordination, and earthwork quantity calculation tasks respectively.

Figure 9
figure 9

Practitioner's preferences for planning steel erection sequence.

Figure 10
figure 10

Practitioner's preferences for quantity takeoff of concrete for slab placement.

Figure 11
figure 11

Practitioner's preferences for planning MEP piping runs.

Figure 12
figure 12

Practitioner's preferences for calculating cut and fill earthwork quantities.

For the steel erection sequence plan, subjects preferred the 3D computer model 58% of the time, 2D drawings 23%, and a physical model 19%. Literature suggests the 2D drawings would be preferred as it gives a proper viewing of relative positioning of the steel members. A 3D computer model would distort distances due to projective ambiguity and does not provide addition information that would be desirable. In addition, the practitioners did not perform a simple steel erection sequence during the task completion. It would be a reasonable assumption that a more complex project with more moving parts would prove even more difficult.

When calculating concrete quantities for a slab placement, 62% of practitioners preferred using 2D drawings compared 38% preferring a 3D computer model and 0% for a physical model. This task requires shape understanding and understanding of necessary dimensional properties, which makes a 3D computer model a superior choice. Given this information, subjects likely prefer the 2D drawings due to their limited experiences with CAD technologies. In the current CAD software packages, a concrete slab element can be clicked on and exact quantities will immediately be presented. Without this knowledge and experience, practitioners revert to their familiarity with quantity takeoffs from two dimensional drawings.

With the need for depth cues, shape understanding, and avoidance of projective ambiguity, coordinating the locations of mechanical, electrical, and plumbing pipes is a demanding task. On one hand, depth cues and shape understanding require a 3D display, while a standard 3D display presents issues of projective ambiguity. The issue is averted in a physical model where subjects benefit from depth cues and shape understanding of a 3D display and avoiding projective ambiguity from a true three dimensional, haptic output. However, 77% of practitioners preferred a 3D computer model, while 15% and 8% chose 2D drawings and a physical model respectively.

Calculating cut and fill earthwork quantities requires a knowledge of the terrain and layout, and ideally, the ability to quickly calculate volumes. 3D CAD software packages are readily equipped with this capability and provide a 3D display that is optimal to complete the task. 58% of practitioners appropriately identify the 3D computer model as the information format of choice for this operation, while 42% would use the standard 2D drawing set and 0% would reference a physical model.

When these responses are aggregated (see Figure 13), 58% of practitioners would use a 3D model for the construction tasks. 34% and 8% would use 2D drawings and a physical model respectively. These numbers are interesting, as the results of the experimental task showed that practitioners perform better with a physical model. Practitioners had difficulty manipulating the computer model to a proper and efficient orientation. In fact, several practitioners could not turn the computer model towards a desired display and ended up turning their work platform to match the orientation on the screen. Furthermore, the participants indicated a significantly higher NASA Composite Workload Demand with the 3D computer model than either of the 2D or physical model (Table 3). With this much difficulty with a simple structural model, a more complex and layered computer model, as are the ones currently populating the industry, would prove to be too burdensome and laborious for efficient field interpretation. Despite the participants' difficulty in manipulating the 3D model, the participants still indicated a significantly stronger preference for using 3D digital information to perform these common construction tasks. Moving forward, these suggests provide compelling data that improving the delivery of 3D digital information to construction crews is both desired and would be beneficial in improving craft worker performance.

Figure 13
figure 13

Practitioners' preferences for all engineering and construction tasks.

Conclusion

The current work tested subjects using various formats of information delivery (2D, 3D interface, and a physical model) to complete a building task and evaluated performance through cognitive and productivity measures. This paper's primary contribution to the body of knowledge is identification of how different mediums of engineering information influences the performance of construction task execution. By conducting a cognitive task experiment, the performance and preferences of subjects with a 3D computer interface and a physical, haptic model was evaluated.

The cognitive workload mean scores were lower with the physical model than the two dimensional drawings and three dimensional computer model average composite workload. These findings suggest that 3D printed models present a promising alternative as at lease a supplemental form of spatial information to the traditional forms of information delivery. There are forms of engineering information, such as dimensions and material specifications, that are impractical to be imposed on a 3D printed model with current printing capabilities. While a 3D computer model may be a more convenient method to representing 3D design due to its current prevalence in industry, it does not appear to be an effective method for spatial designs. This can negate the benefits of the third dimension, since space is the critical addition to the information presentation.

For construction practitioners, the current work implies that physical models present a potential alternative for spatial representation than a 3D computer model and even 2D drawings for spatial tasks. The physical models have higher direct work rates and lower composite workload scores than a 3D computer model and 2D drawings. The task dependent nature of information displays, along with the performance of subjects in the experiment suggest specific situations control the advantages and disadvantages of various mediums. For example, 2D drawings can be useful in tasks involving layouts and relative positioning of objects. A 3D computer model can be useful for dimensioning 3D elements, repetitive calculations, and shape properties due to the intelligence contained within the model. Physical models provide an advantage for tasks that require visualization of spatial elements, coordination of space, and a scaled understanding of depth such as critical lift planning, structure steel sequencing, and planning staging areas for material. Limitations of the current work include a limited sample size and the limited applicability of the model building task to an actual construction task. With a larger sample size and replication of actual construction tasks, the implications for construction practitioners become more impactful. Future work in this area includes further analysis of mental workload in construction professionals to begin the process of understanding the cognitive work demands associated with construction tasks. The outcomes of such studies can identify components of tasks that are mentally demanding and inhibit understanding and execution of the task efficiently. Furthermore, there is a need to examine the use of 3D printed models for more complex designs that can be physically assembled and dissembled by craft workers and other end users and the influence this would have on construction performance. This in essence would replicate the function that traditional modeler built physical models provided on jobsites decades ago. In addition, the current cost to 3D print a project model is substantial, and the print sizes can be relatively small. A cost-benefit analysis on the use of a 3D printed model as a means of information presentation is needed.

References

  • Berlo DK: The Process of Communication: An Introduction to Theory and Practice. The University of Michigan, New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borcherding S, Samelson : Improving motivation and productivity on large projects. Journal of the Construction Division 1980, 106: No. CO1, 77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer BS, Wickens CD: 3D weather displays for aircraft cockpits. Aviation Research Laboratory, Savoy, IL; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breen J, Nottrot R, Stellingwerff M: Tangible virtuality - perceptions of computer-aided and physical modelling. Journal of Automation in Construction, Elsevier 2003, 12: 649–653. 10.1016/S0926-5805(03)00053-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bureau of Economic Analysis. (2013). National Income and Product Accounts Tables. U.S. Department of Commerce. (July 16th, 2013)., [http://www.bea.gov/iTable/iTable.cfm?ReqID=9&step=1#reqid=9&step=3&isuri=1&903=317]

  • Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2012). Overview of BLS Productivity Statistics. U.S. Department of Labor. (April 21st, 2013)., [http://www.bls.gov/bls/productivity.htm]

  • Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2013). News Release: Multifactor Productivity Trends for Detailed Industries, 2011 (USDL-13-1941). U.S. Department of Labor. ., [http://www.bls.gov/news.release/pdf/prin3.pdf]

  • Buswell RA, Soar RC, Gibb AGF, Thorpe A: Freeform construction: mega-scale rapid manufacturing for construction. Journal of Automation in Construction, Elsevier 2007, 16: 224–231. 10.1016/j.autcon.2006.05.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byers JC, Bittner AC, Hill SG: “Traditional and raw task load index (TLX) correlations: Are paired comparisons necessary?”. In Advances in Industrial Ergonomics and Safety. Edited by: Mital A. Taylor and Francis, London; 1989:481–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carswell CM, Clarke D, Seales WB: Assessing mental workload during laparoscopic surgery. Surgical Innovation 2005, 12(1):80–90. 10.1177/155335060501200112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cockburn A, McKenzie B: Evaluating the Effectiveness of Spatial Memory in 2D and 3D Physical and Virtual Environments. In CHI 2002. ACM, New York, Minneapolis, MN; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, K, & Lee, G. (2009). “Managing the Dynamics of Projects and Changes at Fluor.” Conference of the System Dynamics Society. In Proceedings from the 27th International Conference of the System Dynamics Society (pp. 1-27). Albuquerque, New Mexico.

  • Dadi GB, Goodrum PM, Taylor TRB, Carswell CM: Cognitive workload demands using 2D and 3D spatial engineering information formats. Journal of Construction Engineering and Management 2014, 140(5):04014001. 10.1061/(ASCE)CO.1943-7862.0000827

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dai, J, Goodrum, PM, Maloney, WF, & Sayers, C. (2005). Analysis of Focus Group Data Regarding Construction Craft Workers' Perspective of the Factors Affecting Their Productivity. In Proceedings from the 2005 Construction Research Congress (pp. 1-10). Published by the American Society of Civil Engineers.

  • Dai J, Goodrum PM, Maloney WF: Construction craft workers' perceptions of the factors affecting their productivity. Journal of Construction Engineering and Management 2009, 135(3):217–226. 10.1061/(ASCE)0733-9364(2009)135:3(217)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dai J, Goodrum PM, Maloney WF, Srinivasan C: Latent structures of the factors affecting construction labor productivity. Journal of Construction Engineering and Management 2009, 135(5):397–406. 10.1061/(ASCE)0733-9364(2009)135:5(397)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dielman TE: Applied Regression Analysis: a Second Course in Business and Economic Statistics 4th ed. South-Western Cengage Learning, Mason, OH; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckert C, Boujut JF: The role of objects in design co-operation: communication through physical or virtual objects. Computer Supported Cooperative Work 2003, 12: 145–151. 10.1023/A:1023954726209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emmitt S, Gorse C: Construction Communication. Blackwell Publishing, Oxford; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fellows R, Liu A: Research Methods for Construction. Wiley-Blackwell Publishing, Malden, MA; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodrum PM, Dai (CII) J: Work Force View of Construction Productivity. The Construction Industry Institute, University of Texas at Austin; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodrum PM, Haas CT, Glover R: The divergence in aggregate and activity estimates of U.S. construction productivity. Journal of Construction Management and Economics E & F N Spon 2002, 20(5):415–423. 10.1080/01446190210145868

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould F, Joyce N: Construction Project Management. Prentice Hall, New Jersey; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haas CT, Goodrum PM, Caldas CH, Granger R, Zhang D: A Guide to Construction Rework Reduction. The Construction Industry Institute, University of Texas at Austin; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart SG: “NASA-task load index (NASA-TLX); 20 years later”. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics 50th Annual Meeting. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica, CA; 2006:904–908.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart SG, Staveland LE: Development of the NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): Results of empirical and theoretical research. 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hickox JG, Wickens CD: Effects of elevation angle disparity, complexity and feature type on relating out-of-cockpit field of view to an electronic cartographic map. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 1999, 5: 284–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huesman, J, Holland, L, & Langley, T. (2013). December 2012 Construction at $885.0 Billion Annual Rate. U.S. Census Bureau News. U.S. Department of Commerce. . (July 16th, 2013)., [http://www.census.gov/construction/c30/pdf/pr201212.pdf]

  • Lim S, Buswell RA, Le TT, Austin SA, Gibb AGF, Thorpe T: Developments in construction-scale additive manufacturing processes. Journal of Automation in Construction, Elsevier 2012, 21: 262–268. 10.1016/j.autcon.2011.06.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitropoulos P, Memarian B: Task demands in masonry work: sources, performance implications and management strategies. Journal of Construction Engineering and Management. 2013, 139(5):581–590. 10.1061/(ASCE)CO.1943-7862.0000586

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moroney WF, Biers DW, Eggemeier FT, Mitchell JA: A comparison of two scoring procedures with the NASA Task Load Index in a simulated flight tasks. Proceedings from NAECON, Dayton, OH; 1992.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moroney WF, Biers DW, Eggemeier FT: Some measurement and methodological considerations in the application of subjective workload measurement techniques. International Journal of Aviation Psychology. 1995, 5(1):87–106. 10.1207/s15327108ijap0501_6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mourgues, C, & Fischer, M. (2008). A Work Instruction Template for Cast-in-Place Concrete Construction Laborers: Center for Integrated Facility Engineering. Stanford University. Working Paper #109.

  • O'Brien B, Leite F, Meeks S: Enhanced Work Packaging: Design through Workface Execution. The Construction Industry Institute, University of Texas at Austin; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oglesby CH, Parker HW, Howell GA: Productivity improvement in construction. McGraw-Hill, New York; 1989.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rojas EM: Construction productivity: a practical guide for building and electrical contractors. J. Ross Publications, Fort Lauderdale; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schramm W: The Process and Effects of Communication. University of Illinois Press, Champaign, IL; 1954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon CE, Weaver W: The Mathematical Theory of Communication. University of Illinois Press, Champaign, IL; 1949.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Simondetti A: Computer-generated physical modelling in the early stages of the design process. Journal of Automation in Construction, Elsevier 2002, 11: 303–311. 10.1016/S0926-5805(00)00105-9

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • St John M, Cowen MB, Smallman HS, Oonk HM: The use of 2D and 3D displays for shape-understanding versus relative-position tasks. Human Factors 2001, 43(1):79–98. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 10.1518/001872001775992534

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor TRB, Uddin M, Goodrum PM, McCoy A, Shan Y: Change orders and lessons learned: knowledge from statistical analyses of engineering change orders on Kentucky highway projects. Journal of Construction Engineering and Management, ASCE 2012, 138(12):1360–1369. 10.1061/(ASCE)CO.1943-7862.0000550

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gabriel B Dadi.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

GBD conducted the literature review, administered the experiments, collected the data, and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. PMG revised the manuscript for intellectual content, was instrumental in the concept of the experiment, and assisted in the design of the experiment. TRBT revised the manuscript for intellectual content, assisting in drafting the scope of the manuscript, and assisted in the design of the experiment. WFM revised the manuscript for intellectual content and assisted in the design of the experiment. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits use, duplication, adaptation, distribution, and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dadi, G.B., Goodrum, P.M., Taylor, T.R. et al. Effectiveness of communication of spatial engineering information through 3D CAD and 3D printed models. Vis. in Eng. 2, 9 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40327-014-0009-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40327-014-0009-8

Keywords