Skip to main content

Graphical interface design for children seeking information in a digital library

Abstract

Background

Children are very curious. They like to play games but lack reading fluency. Conventional interfaces for information-seeking in digital libraries are unsuitable for children because they are rigid, text-based, and task-oriented. Thus, this study developed a customized interface for a digital library. To facilitate children in online searches, the design concepts included user-friendly target icons, icon recognition, and way-finding in a two-dimensional visualized environment.

Methods

In this study, the criteria used to develop visualization tools that children can use for information retrieval were based on theories of human perception related to graphical interfaces for information visualization systems, including iconic representations and information density on visualized displays. Databases widely used by children in the National Library of Public Information were linked to the suggested database frame. The database frame was classified into five major subject categories: Natural Sciences, Fine Arts and Music, Health and Athletics, History and Culture, and Treasure of Knowledge. The web interface was designed according to way-finding concepts and map-structure hierarchies. Information searches in the main frame, sub-frames, and linked database were designed to be simple and intuitive for use by children.

Results

Information search efficiency was measured in 104 children assigned to search the virtual environment for icons (databases) that adequately supported their studies. Statistical analyses showed that, compared to the text-lined interface, the graphical interface improved the success and speed of searches by children.

Conclusions

Incorporating theories of spatial memory and representative icons in designs for visualized retrieval systems helps children to recognize different categories of knowledge content and improves their efficiency in information retrieval. To improve database search efficiency in children, information recommendation issues require further study. However, further research is also needed to identify factors that affect patterns of information-seeking behavior in children.

Background

Information-seeking behaviors substantially differ between children and adults. In a comparison of web search patterns between children and graduate students, Bilal and Kirby (2002) found that the latter had markedly higher search effectiveness and markedly higher web navigation efficiency and quality. Graduate students were also more proficient in recovering from search related “breakdowns.” Therefore, current information-searching interfaces, which are usually designed for adults, may be ineffective for children.

Many studies agree that, compared to adults, children have limited information seeking capability and require an improved web interface to help them overcome the information searching barrier. Kulper et al. (1997) found that most children aged 8–11 years cannot perform a search by author name. Other difficulties included spelling or phrasing search statements, applying the search terms used in official subject headings, narrowing the search fields, and using Boolean expressions. Children also tend to stop searching after a few hits. Kulper et al. (1997) then concluded that, in databases designed for children, the icons used in the interface should have simple and clear meanings and should be used as a benchmark for orientating children in further searching. Kulper et al. (1997) further used SoftWare Technique for Evolutionary Participatory System development (STEP) to develop “BUCHERSCHATZ” (book treasure) and an OPAC prototype with a graphical user interface (GUI).

Hutchinson et al. (2005) studied the errors and frustrations that limit information-seeking effectiveness in children. For example, some children could not spell, type, read, navigate, compose queries, and/or select small objects. The terms used in book searches differed between children and adults. Since children have difficulty manipulating and understanding conventional interface tools, their searching and browsing strategies often differ from those applied by adults. Based on this finding, Hutchinson et al. (2007) designed a category browser that uses Boolean queries, colorful icons, and a flat-structured, simultaneous-response interface to help children search for information in the International Children Digital Library (ICDL).

The Internet Public Library (IPL) is hosted by the College of Information Science & Technology at Drexel University. Drexel University also hosts the ipl2 FOR KIDs, which provides web-links to useful information for young users to do homework as well as leisure reading. Its nine search categories include Reference, Our World, Computer & Internet, Health & Nutrition, Reading Zone, Math & Science, Art & Music, Sports & Recreation, and Fun Stuff (Drexel University 2012).

Beheshti (2012) designed and tested a virtual information environment for children. The design of the virtual library environment was based on the metaphor of a physical library with rooms, bookshelves and books. Users can scan and open books on virtual shelves and can even use search stations to conduct conventional keyword searches. Book locations are identified by red dots on the floor plan for the virtual library.

Most conventional information-seeking interfaces are ineffective for use by children because they are rigid, text-based, and task-oriented. In his classic study, Solomon (1993) examined four factors that affect the information retrieval behavior of children when the using online public access catalog (OPAC): user characteristics, school setting, interface usability, and information access features. In Table 1, four GUI information-seeking interfaces designed for children are compared in terms of user behavior, interface type/display of information node and icon design, and collection type. While these GUI interfaces were designed specifically for children, they require improvements in intuiting user behavior, categorizing information, and providing a game-like search environment. Therefore, this work presents a customized interface for a digital library with novel design concepts (i.e., the avoidance of conjunction syntax operations and the use of icon recognition, a user-friendly icon layout, a search-target avatar, and way-finding approach in a two-dimensional visualized environment) to facilitate online searches by children.

Table 1 Comparisons of four GUI interface designed for children

Methods

A graphical interface for children seeking information contained in the National Library of Public Information (NLPI, renamed from National Taichung Library) database is developed by applying a user-centered design concept. First, literature review is performed to determine how children use information node layouts and icon representation-related topics, which are essential for effective use of a visualized database and its spatial arrangement. Five design guidelines for the information-seeking interface are then established: (a) providing children with an effective way-finding approach to searching information nodes in a virtual environment, (b) allowing gradual access to the graphic display by spreading out the search targets in an extended virtual reality environment, (c) using icons based on simplified images relevant to the experience of children to allow them to perceive the actual meaning of the database content, (d) integrating both text and relevant images into an icon to represent a database in a digital library to help children recognize its content, and (e) establishing a game-like information search environment that children would enjoy using for information search rather than a work-like and classroom-like machine interface.

The interface prototype is then implemented using the cognitive walkthrough method, which is widely used to develop digital libraries for children, to study educational development in children, and to categorize and visualize information. Based on expert suggestions in each development step, the scope of the database searches are designed to meet the requirements of children completing school homework and leisure reading assignments. Icons are also designed for five different databases categories: Natural Sciences, Fine Arts and Music, Health and Athletics, History and Culture, and Treasure of Knowledge. Finally, Flash software is used to embed all icons in a left-right-extended interface prototype so that users can perform search tasks in experimental tests of interface usability.

Information visualization and density

Although children enjoy searching through icons in play-oriented information visualization interfaces to retrieve information, several topics related to information visualization and iconic representation in GUI design need further study. Shneiderman (1997) reported that the development of window management interfaces should prioritize information visualization and user interfaces. Since the literacy skills of children improve slowly before adolescence, the use of search strategies that combine text with images vary by age. Therefore, information systems require interfaces with improved visual communication properties, including iconic representation and arrangement (metaphor), to help children during the information-seeking process.

Hu et al. (1999) suggested that interface design may substantially affect system–user concept communication, regardless of user familiarity with the search task; for facilitating such communication, they also reported that a GUI is more effective than a list-based design. For supporting concept communications, interfaces that incorporate multiple visual properties may be more effective than those that rely exclusively on visual properties. Although using graphical interfaces and text-based linear lists incur similar cognitive loads, graphical interfaces reportedly provide higher user satisfaction. The importance of user satisfaction with the interface increases in designs with multiple visual properties. Improvements in interface-induced system-user concept communication may increase with the number of system-suggested items but decrease with user task familiarity. Further, differences in size and distance may provide many effective and essential visual cues in system–user concept communication whereas differences in color provides relatively less effective visual cues for such communication support. The findings of Hu et al. (1999) regarding the visual properties that help users communicate with information systems motivated this study of a novel two-dimensional spatial-organized interface for information systems to help children perform search tasks.

Shneiderman (2003) stated that an object can be characterized by using some aspects of the data in the object to indicate its position. Koshman (2006) reviewed the literature on visualization systems used for information retrieval. She analyzed iconic representations and information density in visual displays. Whereas concepts from human perception theory, including proximity, closure, and connectivity, are applicable in graphical displays (e.g., user overviews of large collections in a digital database on limited-scale displays; how collections (presented as symbols), an improved understanding of how information density can be organized into different layouts and level structures to help users identify the structural contents (metaphors) of a database and to obtain accurate search results that can be presented on a screen.

In web-based visualization systems for adults, data are usually spatially organized into a network or polar system in which the information nodes are web sites and databases. The path between two nodes indicates the queried relationship between two databases or the sequential searching actions of web users. For example, the Mapstan.com web tool records collective search results by fixing a map-path width (weight) inferred from the number of web-visited users between two nodes (databases). The relationship between two nodes is clearly defined and implied the content of nearby database groups. Since children are naturally curious and enjoy exploring unknown worlds, they may not use the most efficient search procedure. For example, they may use trial-and-error or fuzzy searches of possible target groups to find their target. Thus, enabling children to apply a free way-finding approach to searching an information node in the virtual environment is the first design guideline.

In the first version of the ICDL, Reuter and Druin (2004) reported that elementary school students preferred navigating digital libraries hierarchically (less cognitive load) instead of by using Boolean searches. Hutchinson et al. (2007) redesigned the category browser by using simultaneous menus to allow book searches within minor hyperlink levels (less memory load). Compared to a multi-level hyperlink structure, a board and extended plan structure allows children to search a virtual reality environment more efficiently and without getting lost. Thus, the second design guideline is to distribute the search targets in an extended virtual reality environment so that children can search targets as they gradually move across the graphic display (background). This approach provides visual cues that help prevent children from getting lost in a sectional, breakdown condition. If the user reaches the wrong node, the cues in the virtual environmental help the user find the original path and find the nodes with similar meanings.

Iconic representations

Users must apply information visualization tools to interpret and decode icons and to judge whether the retrieval results are useful. According to Koshman (2006), p.205, a weakness of the web visualized tool is its possible arbitrary assignments of iconic representations to databases with inappropriate content. Even if the map metaphor helps users navigate and identify location parameters, it may not enable them to retrieve desired information represented with meaningful icons. Since the ICDL was developed for English speakers, Bilal and Bachir (2007) stressed the importance of the ‘design representations’ and ‘meaning’ of information for non-native English-speaking children who use ICDL. They also reported that older children perform book-searching tasks more efficiently than younger children do. Their objective was to incorporate the meanings and representations of icons in a child-friendly visual interface for information search.

Hu et al. (1999) described the visual properties that improve communication between users and information systems but recommended further study of contextual and emotional issues. Bates (2002) addressed contextual issues related to interface design and reported that an adequate design requires a clear understanding of cascade interactions between the user and system and that each system requires a different interface. Bilal (2001, 2002) suggested that emotions can profoundly impact the overall effectiveness of children in using a Web search engine to perform assigned and fully self-generated search tasks. Several studies have assessed systems that communicate effect by incorporating emotionally expressive computer agents. Leide et al. (2003) mapped the search process used by domain-novice users in a visualized IR system and applied benchmark (lighthouse) concept index user knowledge boundaries in classification-based visualization schemes. Tzeng (2004) examined the effects of text and emoticons used to express sympathy for failures in computer games and reported that apologetic (sad) emoticons communicate emotions more effectively compared to pure text. Brave et al. (2005) studied how emotionally expressive computer agents affect the perceptions of players during computer games. Participants perceived empathetic agents as likeable, trustworthy, supportive, and beneficial. In addition to reviewing some of the human perceptual theories underlying graphical interfaces for information visualization systems, Schmidt and Stock (2009) applied a similar method of using tags to express basic emotions such as anger, disgust, fear, happiness and sadness in images.

Whereas most related studies have attempted to use standardized abstract-format icons and even text to represent important meanings in a visualized system, children generally ascribe meanings to symbols based on actual objects in their environment. For children, learning a new set of visualized symbols is harder than identifying images they observe in daily life. Thus, the third design guideline is using icons based on simplified images relevant to the experience of children to allow them to perceive the actual meaning of the database content.

Morse et al. (2002) compared visual information retrieval methodologies that used textual, iconic, graphical, and “spring” displays and found that the textual interface provided the highest user performance (p. 35); for the most complex tasks, however, graphical methods (icon, graphs, “spring”) were superior. In Heo and Hirtle (2001), a study of how users process large quantities of information presented in a graphical interface revealed that the “expanding outline” is the most effective presentation. Icons, whether they are text or images, may help children of varying age and of varying literacy understand the meaning of an icon. Therefore, the fourth design guideline is to use both text and images as icons to help children understand the content of a digital library.

Blom and Monk (2003) suggested that users have various motivations and emotional reasons for personalizing their computers, including increasing their sense of control and ownership, increasing their enjoyment, and relieving boredom. Zhang (2008) proposed that the three mental models students use for web searches are acquiring information, understanding the working mechanisms of search engines, and devising search tactics. Lopatovska and Arapakis (2011) developed affective systems that can recognize and appropriately respond to human emotions and, ultimately, increase the effectiveness and pleasure of human–computer interaction (HCI). Thus, the fifth guideline is establishing a fun, game-like, and intuitive environment for seeking information instead of a work-like, classroom-like, machine-like interface that children are likely to find boring.

According to the Gibson Affordance Theory, humans perceive possibilities (affordances) for action in order to operate in the environment. Perception is designed for action. Yu (2012) explained that visual perception affects how children categorize and narrate information. A visual interface design that children can use effectively for searching information must include appropriate affordances to simplify the task. According to a study of information-seeking behavior by Shenton & Dixon (2004), children tend to spend minimum effort to find sufficient information, they apply the same method repeatedly for a special information need, they select the search strategy at the point of need, and they apply untaught, expedient information-seeking methods. Thus, we argue that an effective visualized searching interface must enable repeated use of simple search strategies and must enable intuitive way-finding to the right information node in an extended virtual reality environment. Paivio (2006) introduced the dual-coding theory that visual and verbal information are processed differently and along distinct channels in the human mind. Thus, we argue that the iconic representations and the layouts of visual displays are critical for helping children to interpret and choose correct icons (information nodes) in different visual and textual forms to meet children recognition and living experiences. Most importantly, the visualized interface stimulates attracts the interest of the user.

Interface prototype development

This study developed a set of guidelines for using an icon-searching interface and a spatial-linked structure for On-line Databases used in the NLPI for Children. The core concept is to present the meaning searching process as a treasure-hunting game by using the virtual reality tool to create a graphical interface prototype, which children can use to search for information databases (icons) in the virtual reality environment. Since NLPI is responsible for information services used by children in Taiwan public libraries, it collects many digital databases used by children. Given the many digital libraries established for children worldwide, the NLPI also plans to establish a user-friendly interface for a digital library for children in Taiwan. Notably, the original text-based interface (Figure 1) for the NLPI digital library for children has already been changed to a graphical interface (Figure 2). The proposed interface has a two-dimensional virtual space that integrates different icons connected to different databases. After receiving approval from the service librarians and NLPI director, this study identified the databases that children typically use for general search (Table 2).

Figure 1
figure 1

The NLPI text-based interface for children digital library.

Figure 2
figure 2

The NLPI graphical interface for children digital library.

Table 2 NLPI databases linked to icons

Figure 3 shows how the extended graphical interface uses icons to represent databases linked to the NLPI. Examples of databases include Copernicus 21 Sciences Knowledge database, Taiwan Amphibious Reptile video database, Taiwan Ecology Notebook database, etc. Based on guidelines 1 and 2, this study developed an extended visualized environment with icons representing different databases to enable children to search for information without getting lost in multiple hyperlink hierarchies.

Figure 3
figure 3

Extended graphical design for the interface of NLPI children digital library.

Concepts of way-finding and map-structure hierarchy were applied in the design of the graphical interface. Since children can typically conceptualize only seven categories, the databases were divided into five subject category groups - Natural Sciences, Fine Arts and Music, Health and Athletics, History and Culture, and Treasure of Knowledge, which are the subjects included in the primary school curriculum. For example, the Natural Sciences category group contained three lower-level icons: Science Knowledge, Exploring Ecology, and Sciences Experiments. Each lower-level icon was then linked to several databases, and each database was presented in a separate window frame (Table 2).

To search for information in the designed mainframe, the user moves the cursor (the spaceman) right or left (Figure 4) in the map to select an icon that represents a category. When the user clicks on the icon for the chosen database, a small window frame presents the detailed content of the database. Figure 5 displays how the up and down arrows can be used to navigate through different window frames to the target database. A video demonstrating how children use the proposed graphical interface for information search is available online (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=WDLWYfOpg5Y&feature=youtu.be).Human perceptual theory, which underlies the development of graphical interfaces of information visualization systems, was applied in the design of the interface by using iconic representations that are easily recognized by children. Since simplified images relevant to the experiences of children enables them to recognize the database content, 19 images were selected (Figure 6). For example, visual arts are represented by paint brushes and color plates; Taiwan history is represented by the national flag.

Figure 4
figure 4

The graphical interface for children digital library with a pop-up window frame.

Figure 5
figure 5

Operations in the pop-up window frame. The up-down arrows are used to move among different databases, after children entering the window frame.

Figure 6
figure 6

Icons in five category groups.

The icons comprised images associated with the text and phonetic notation in order to help children recognize the database content. Since skills such as literacy, image recognition, and information searching develop at different rates, most children use an integrated method to deduce the content of various databases.

Results and discussion

User-oriented evaluation

Genuis (2004) categorized usability evaluations as expert-oriented or user-oriented. Users do not participate in the process applied by experts to assess usability. Therefore, the expert-oriented assessment process eventually causes a mismatch between assessment results and actual user conditions. In the user-oriented evaluation process, researchers collect data when users are performing tasks. However, since the assessment results in this study need to be consistent with the usability requirements, this work also applied the Usability Testing method of user-oriented evaluation to investigate how children use the designed graphical interface and the original text-based interface. While the children performed search tasks on the assigned interfaces, the researchers recorded their behavior in each step. Finally, the researchers analyzed the users in terms of success rates, operating speeds, and satisfaction to improve the interface usability (Rubin 1994).

Figure 7 shows the relationships among the evaluation factors, including children (user) characteristics, searching interfaces, and interface usability. Children characteristic includes grade (age) and computer usage. In a study of interaction design for children, Hourcade (2007) described visual acuity in static and dynamic (moving objects) settings. Static visual acuity rapidly improves from ages 5–7 years and from ages 9–10 years and is usually mature by age 10 years. Dynamic visual acuity similarly improves until ages 11 and 12 years. Thus, the selected age distribution of the sample was age 7 to 11 years. While experts suggest 30–45 minutes of free play for elementary school children with extra time allotted for school work (Matte 2014), this research defined heavy and light computer usage as more than 4 hours per week and less than 4 hours per week, respectively.

Figure 7
figure 7

Relationships among experimental factors used to test children with different characteristics.

The searching interface includes the original text-listed interface and the suggested graphical interface. Interface usability was evaluated in terms of success rate and operating speed. Success rate was defined as the proportion of children who reached the target icon in a test divided by the total number of children assigned to the group. Operating speed is defined as the time children required to complete each task. The graphical interface incorporating visual memory and iconic representation into visualized retrieval systems clearly helped the children recognize different categories of knowledge content. However, further statistical confirmation of improved information searching efficiency is needed.

Experiment and sampling

The experiment was performed at the NLPI. The 104 students were randomly divided into two groups in order to compare the original NLPI text-based interface and the graphical interface prototype in terms of interface usability. Table 3 shows that 23 students were in grades 1–2, 46 were in grades 3–4, and 35 were in grades 5–6. Additionally, computer usage experience was classified as light (less than 4 hours per week) in 83 students and heavy (more than 4 hours per week) in 21 students.

Table 3 Samples in grade and computer usage experience

Before beginning the experiment, the instructor demonstrated the operation of the interface to each child in the two groups and then introduced two tasks sequentially. The children were required to complete the task in 90 seconds and to inform the instructor after completing each task so that the instructor could record their times. The following two information search tasks were used in the experiment.

You encounter a problem while doing homework and your instructor tells you to search the online Taiwan Historical Dictionary database. You are now trying to find this database.

The second task was as follows:

Search the green sour bear picture books database after school to find books for leisure reading.

Hypotheses for interface comparison

The graphical interface and the original text-based interface were compared in terms of success rates and operating speeds for information searches. As the children were divided randomly to avoid interference between sequential operations performed by each group, Pearson chi-squared test was used to compare success rates between the two interface groups. Two-way analysis of variance was applied to examine the influence of two categorical independent variables (grade-interface or computer usage-interface) on one dependent variable (operating speed). We hypothesized the following:

H1: Regardless of grade, success rates for search tasks differ between the two interfaces.

H2: Regardless of grade, children who successfully complete the information search tasks use the two interfaces at different operating speeds.

H3: Regardless of computer usage experience, success rates for search tasks differ between the two interfaces.

H4: Regardless of computer usage experience, children who successfully complete the information search tasks use the two interfaces at different operating speeds.

Comparison of grade levels

Table 4 shows the task 1 results. Performance significantly differed between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface in all grade levels (grades 1–2 (**p = .006 < .01); grades 3–4 (***p = .000 < .001); grades 5–6 (**p = .003 < .01)). Regardless of grade, success rates for task 1 were higher for the graphical interface than for the original text-based interface. Success rates for task 2 also significantly differed between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface in all grade levels (grades 1–2 (***p = .000 < .001), grades 3–4 (***p = .000 < .001), and grades 5–6 (***p = .000 < .001)). In task 2, success rates were higher in all grades of children who used the graphical interface than in those who used the original text-based interface. Hypothesis H1 is thus validated.

Table 4 Comparisons of success rates between graphical interface and text-based linear lists interface

Regardless of grade, children who successfully completed the information search tasks used the two interfaces at different operating speeds. Table 5 shows that, regardless of grade, success in completing task 1 significantly differed (**p = .005 < .01) between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface. Regardless of grade, successful completion of Task 2 also significantly (***p = . 000 < .001) differed between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface. In all grades, the average operating speed (time required to complete the task) when using the graphical interface was faster (shorter) than that using the original text-based interface. Hypothesis H2 is thus validated.

Table 5 Comparisons of operating speeds between graphical interface and text-based linear lists interface

Comparison of computer usage experience

Table 6 shows that success in completing task 1 significantly differed between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface in students with light computer usage experience (***p = .000 < .001) and in those with heavy computer usage experience (*p = .011 < .05). Regardless of computer usage experience, success rates for task 1 were higher in students who used the graphical interface than in those who used the original text-based interface. Success in completing task 2 significantly differed between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface in students with light computer usage experience (***p = .000 < .001) and in those with heavy computer usage experience (**p = .005 < .01). Regardless of computer usage experience, success rates for task 2 were higher in students who used the graphical interface than in those who used the original text-based interface. Hypothesis H3 is thus validated.

Table 6 Comparisons of success rates between graphical interface and text-based linear lists interface for children with different computer usage experiences

At all computer experience levels, the children who successfully completed the information search tasks used the two interfaces at different operating speeds. Table 7 shows that success in completing task 1 significantly differed (*p = .021 < .05) between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface, regardless of computer experience. Similarly, success in completing task 2 significantly (***p = .000 < .001) differed between the original text-based interface and the graphical interface, regardless of computer experience. Finally, the average operating speed (time) required to use the graphical interface was faster (shorter) than those required to use the original text-based interface regardless of computer usage experience. Hypothesis H4 is thus validated.

Table 7 Comparisons of operating speeds between graphical interface and text-based linear lists interface for children with different computer usage experiences

This study elucidates information-seeking behavior of children and compares their navigating experience in each interface. The statistical data obtained in comparisons of success rates and operating speeds contribute to efforts to develop an improved interface for emotional information retrieval. The children also had different comments and complaints about the two interfaces. Typical complaints about the text-based interface included of difficulty finding the right keywords, difficulty typing, difficulty reading the small font, the high complexity of the interface, and the ease of performing the wrong operation. In contrast, the children commented that the images used in the graphical interface were attractive and that its icons were organized consistently with the curriculum structure and were associated with phonetic notation, which enabled easy use of trial-and-error methods without breakdown after a failed search.

Differences between children and graduate students

Bilal and Kirby (2002) compared information-seeking behavior between children and graduate students using Yahooligans. They found that graduate students employed advanced search syntax, but the children did not; graduate students applied a “linear or systematic” browsing style to locate information whereas most children had a “loopy” style; graduate students made fewer Web moves, used fewer hyperlinks, and made fewer backtracks than children did; graduate students did not deviate from any designated targets whereas most children did; graduate students could quickly recover “breakdowns” in keyword searches whereas children could not.

Since the graphical interface improved the effectiveness of information searches by children, this study further evaluated the interface in graduate students. In two tests performed in six graduate students, the average time needed to complete task 1 was 29.67 seconds (range, 13 to 83 seconds), and the average time needed to complete task 2 was 33.83 seconds (range, 22 to 48 seconds). The graduate students tended to view each icon quickly and sequentially to find the first target (online Taiwan Historical Dictionary of databases), and they tended to remember the icons they reviewed. In task 2 (finding green sour bear picture books database), all students were confused about the meaning of recently viewed Visual Arts icons, and none performed backtrack searches correctly. When they could not find the target, they searched the other side of the extended interface and found the target located on Children Story Collection icon. Since the organization of the icons was consistent with the school curriculum structure, children seldom confused the meanings of the icons. However, topics related to Information Categorization and Information Recommendation both require further study. Although the average operational speeds for graduate students were faster than those for children in both primary examinations, larger samples are needed to confirm the usability differences between children and graduate students (and even adults).

Conclusion

The information-seeking behaviors of children when using interfaces associated with different media differ from those of adults. For example, children perform web searches in different affective states of navigation. This study developed a customized interface to help children overcome obstacles to using the NLPI digital library for information seeking. Comparisons of usage behavior between the existing text-based interface and the new graphical interface show that the latter is preferred by the children who participated in this study.

Since the children indicated that they enjoyed using the NLPI graphical interface, the statistical results provide a valuable reference for considering the effects of visualized information in the design of information retrieval systems. While children are a special user group with unique characteristics, further studies of Information Recommender in children are needed to enhance the usability of interfaces that present information graphically. Many information recommender systems collaboratively evaluate and filter web content and are useful for individual searches of relevant information (Ardoin et al. 2013; Chen et al. 2013; Jafarkarimi et al. 2012; Tejeda-Lorente et al. 2014). To develop appropriate recommendations of information items (textual and graphical), a visual interface must consider how graphical information-seeking behavior differs between children and adults.

In addition to adequate information-seeking skills and intelligent affective strategies, children require customized, enjoyable interfaces to help them surf digital libraries online (Druin 2005; Nesset & Large 2004). To develop an effective graphical interface for a digital library for children, further confirmatory statistical analysis is needed to determine how the information-seeking behavior of children is affected by latent variables such as information type, media type, and affective state during information search.

References

  • Ardoin SP, Christ TJ, Morena LS, Cormier DC, Klingbeil DA: A systematic review and summarization of the recommendations and research surrounding Curriculum-Based Measurement of oral reading fluency (CBM-R) decision rules. Journal of School Psychology 2013, 51(1):1–18. 10.1016/j.jsp.2012.09.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bates MJ: The cascade of interactions in the digital library interface. Information Processing and Management 2002, 38(3):381–400. 10.1016/S0306-4573(01)00041-3

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Beheshti J: Virtual Environments for Children and Teens. In Virtual Reality in Psychological, Medical and Pedagogical Applications Edited by: Eichenberg C. 2012, 271–286. doi:10.5772/51628 doi:10.5772/51628

    Google Scholar 

  • Bilal D: Children’s use of the Yahooligans! Web search engine: II. Cognitive and physical behaviors on research tasks. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2001, 52(2):118–136. 10.1002/1097-4571(2000)9999:9999<::AID-ASI1038>3.0.CO;2-R

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilal D: Children’s use of the Yahooligans! Web search engine. III. Cognitive and physical behaviors on fully self-generated search tasks. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2002, 53(13):1170–1183. doi:10.1002/asi.10145

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilal D, Bachir I: Children’s interaction with cross-cultural and multilingual digital libraries: I. Understanding interface design representations. Information Processing and Management 2007, 43(1):47–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilal D, Kirby J: Differences and similarities in information seeking: children and adults as Web users. Information Processing & Management 2002, 38(5):649–670. doi:10.1016/S0306-4573(01)00057-7

    Article  MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Blom JO, Monk AF: Theory of personalization of appearance: Why users personalize their PCs and mobile phones. Human–Computer Interaction 2003, 18(3):193–228. 10.1207/S15327051HCI1803_1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brave S, Nass C, Hutchinson K: Computers that care: Investigating the effects of orientation of emotion exhibited by an embodied computer agent. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies 2005, 62(2):161–178. 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2004.11.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen J, Zhou X, Jin Q: Recommendation of optimized information seeking process based on the similarity of user access behavior patterns. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing 2013, 17(8):1671–1681. 10.1007/s00779-012-0601-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drexel University: ipl2 For Kids. 2012. http://www.ipl.org/div/kidspace/ Retrieved from

    Google Scholar 

  • Druin A: What children can teach us: Developing digital libraries for children with children. Library Quarterly 2005, 75(1):20–41. 10.1086/428691

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genuis SK: Web site usability testing: A critical tool for libraries. Feliciter 2004, 50(4):161–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heo M, Hirtle S: An empirical comparison of visualization tools to assist information retrieval on the Web. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2001, 52(8):666–675. 10.1002/asi.1117

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hourcade JP: Interaction design and children. Foundation and Trends in Human-Computer Interaction 2007, 1(4):277–392. doi:10.1561/1100000006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu PJ, Ma P, Chau PY: Evaluation of user interface designs for information retrieval systems: a computer-based experiment. Decision Support Systems 1999, 27(1–2):125–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson H, Druin A, Bederson BB, Reuter K, Rose A, Weeks AC: How do I find blue books about dogs? The errors and frustrations of young digital library users. In Proceedings of the 11th International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction (HCII 2005) (CD-ROM). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2005. (Tech Report HCIL-2005–27). Retrieved from University of Maryland, Human-Computer Interaction Lab website: http://hcil.cs.umd.edu/trs/2005–27/2005–27.pdf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson HB, Druin A, Bederson BB: Supporting elementary-age children’s searching and browsing: design and evaluation using the international children’s digital library. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2007, 58(11):1618–1630. 10.1002/asi.20646

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jafarkarimi H, Sim ATH, Saadatdoost R: A naïve recommendation model for large databases. International Journal of Information and Education Technology 2012, 2(3):216–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koshman S: Visualization-based information retrieval on the Web. Library & Information Science Research 2006, 28(2):192–207. 10.1016/j.lisr.2006.03.017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kulper U, Schulz U, Will G: Bucherschatz - A prototype of a children’s OPAC. Information Services & Use, 01675265 1997, 17(2/3):201–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leide JE, Large A, Beheshti J, Brooks M, Cole C: Visualization schemes for domain novices exploring a topic space: the navigation classification scheme. Information Processing and Management 2003, 39(6):923–940. 10.1016/S0306-4573(02)00077-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lopatovska I, Arapakis I: Theories, methods and current research on emotions in library and information science, information retrieval and human–computer interaction. Information Processing and Management 2011, 47(4):575–592. 10.1016/j.ipm.2010.09.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matte C: Limiting computer use for kids. How much time is too much? How do we enforce the rules? Retrieved from. 2014. http://familyinternet.about.com/od/introtofamilycomputing/a/LimitComputer.htm

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse E, Lewis M, Olsen K: Testing visual information retrieval methodologies case study: comparative analysis of textual, icon, graphical and ‘spring’ displays. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2002, 53(1):28–40. 10.1002/asi.10006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nesset V, Large A: Children in the information technology design process: a review of theories and their applications. Library & Information Science Research 2004, 26(2):140–161. 10.1016/j.lisr.2003.12.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paivio A: Mind and its evolution; A dual coding theoretical interpretation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuter K, Druin A: Bringing together children and books: An initial descriptive study of children’s book searching and selection behavior in a digital library. Proceedings of the 67th Annual Meeting of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2004, 339–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin J: Handbook of usability testing: How to plan, design and conduct effective tests. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt S, Stock WG: Collective indexing of emotions in images. A study in emotional information retrieval. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2009, 60(5):863–876. doi:10.1002/asi.21043

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shenton AK, Dixon P: Issues arising from youngsters’ information-seeking behavior. Library & Information Science Research 2004, 26(2):177–200. 10.1016/j.lisr.2003.12.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shneiderman B: The next generation of graphical user interfaces: information visualization and better window management. Displays 1997, 17(3–4):125–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shneiderman B: The eyes have it: A task by data type taxonomy for information visualizations. In The craft of information visualization: Readings and reflections. Edited by: Bedersen B, Shneiderman B. San Francisco: Morgan Kaufmann; 2003:364–371.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon P: Children’s Information Retrieval Behavior: A Case Analysis of an OPAC. Journal of the American Society for Information Science 1993, 44(5):245–264. 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4571(199306)44:5<245::AID-ASI1>3.0.CO;2-#

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tejeda-Lorente A, Porcel C, Peis E, Sanz R, Herrera-Viedma E: A quality based recommender system to disseminate information in a university digital library. Information Sciences 2014, 261: 52–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng JI: Toward a more civilized design: Studying the effects of computers that apologize. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies 2004, 61(3):319–345. 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2004.01.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yu X: Exploring visual perception and children’s interpretations of picture books. Library & Information Science Research 2012, 34(4):292–299. 10.1016/j.lisr.2012.06.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang Y: Undergraduate Students’ Mental Models of the Web as an Information Retrieval System. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology 2008, 59(13):2087–2098. 10.1002/asi.20915

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the National Science Council of the Republic of China, Taiwan for financially/partially supporting this research under Contract No. 99-2515-S-027 -004.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ko-chiu Wu.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

KW carried out the interface analysis studies, participated in the sequence alignment, drafted the manuscript, and directed the entire processes of this study. TY designed the visualized interface. TC programmed the visualized interface. TY and TC undertook the experiments to collect data and analyzed statistical data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 2.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wu, Kc., Tang, Ym. & Tsai, Cy. Graphical interface design for children seeking information in a digital library. Vis. in Eng. 2, 5 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1186/2213-7459-2-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/2213-7459-2-5

Keywords